小黄书

Skip to main content

What does a baby鈥檚 babbling tell you about their future intelligence? More than you think

What does a baby鈥檚 babbling tell you about their future intelligence? More than you think

New 小黄书 Boulder research suggests that tests given as early as 7 months of age can offer a window into what our general cognitive ability may be at age 30 - and beyond.

Watching a baby babble, play and interact with others can provide useful insight into what their cognitive ability might be like decades later, according to new University of Colorado Boulder research .

The study of more than 1,000 twins found that tests as early as 7 months of age can help predict performance on cognitive measures at age 30. It also found that an infant鈥檚 environment plays a more significant role in shaping life-long cognition than scientists realized. It could even influence risk of dementia later in life, the authors said.

鈥淥ur findings highlight the enduring consequences of the very early childhood environment on cognitive ability and suggest that early life is a critical developmental period that we should be paying attention to,鈥 said lead author Daniel Gustavson, assistant research professor at the Institute for Behavioral Genetics (IBG).

What twins can teach us

Similar to 鈥淚Q,鈥 general cognitive ability (GCA), a.k.a. "g factor," is a single, composite measure of a person鈥檚 capacity to learn, reason, understand and problem-solve.

Prior research has shown that much of our GCA is established by childhood: Give an 8-year-old a battery of tests to assess their general intelligence and their score will look remarkably similar at age 30. Measures of IQ at age 20 are highly correlated with those at age 62, and such scores don't change much between age 11 and 90.

But few scientists have looked back further to see what 鈥 if anything鈥攕ignals in infancy can tell us about cognition in adulthood and old age.

Daniel Gustavson

Daniel Gustavson

Gustavson and senior author Chandra Reynolds, a professor of psychology and neuroscience, looked at data from 1,098 participants in the Colorado Longitudinal Twin Study. IBG launched the study in 1985, enrolling baby twins from Colorado鈥檚 Front Range to assess the role that genes vs. environment play in various aspects of development. 听

Researchers have since collected reams of data, via periodic laboratory samples, home visits, surveys, interviews and behavioral tests.

鈥淲e have co-authors on this paper who have been involved since the start and watched these twins grow up,鈥 said Gustavson.

As early as 7 months old, researchers assessed seven measures of cognition, including vocalization, ability to stay on task, and 鈥渘ovelty preference鈥 鈥 whether the infants preferred to play with new toys over ones they were familiar with.

Age-appropriate cognitive assessments have been done at five points, so far.

The team found that looking at cognitive tests in infancy could predict about 13% of the variance in scores at age 30. Two measures 鈥攏ovelty preference and task orientation鈥攚ere the strongest predictors. This early life 鈥渟ignal鈥 is not huge, the authors note.

鈥淲e certainly do not want to imply that cognition is somehow fixed by seven months old,鈥 Gustavson said. 鈥淏ut the idea that a very simple test in infancy can help predict the results of a very complicated cognitive test taken 30 years later is exciting.鈥

Nature, nurture or both?

To explore what role genetics vs. environment plays, the study compared GCA score differences between identical twins, who share 100% of their genes, and fraternal twins, who only share half of their genes. In general, if there is greater similarity among identical twins than fraternal twins, this suggests that genes play a strong role in that trait.

They also analyzed the twins鈥 DNA collected via blood or saliva.

As expected, genes played a big role in influencing general cognitive ability, with genetic influences measured by age 7 accounting for about half of the variation in scores at age 30.

But environment also had a significant and lasting impact.

鈥淥ne of the most exciting findings was that 10% of the variability in adult cognitive ability was explained by environmental influences before year one or two,鈥 said Gustavson.

The older the children got, the more influence genes had and the less environment had.

鈥淭his suggests that even the pre-preschool environment matters,鈥 Gustavson said.

a photo of chandra reynolds

Chandra Reynolds

Reynolds, who studies age-related diseases including Alzheimer鈥檚 and dementia, says the findings could have implications not only for how youth do in school or how adults perform at work but also how prone they may be to age-related cognitive decline.

鈥淐ognitive aging is a life-long process, not just something that begins in mid-life,鈥 she said. 鈥淚t could be that certain interventions, like strong educational foundations in early life could help maximize what people are capable of and help them keep that cognitive gas in the tank for as long as possible.鈥

A polygenic score for intelligence

The study also confirms that 鈥減olygenic scores鈥 can be a useful tool.

Polygenic scores are single numbers that aggregate a person鈥檚 genetic variants to estimate predisposition to a trait, like intelligence.

鈥淭here are thousands of genes that influence intelligence, so you are never going to find an 鈥榠ntelligence gene鈥, but we have found many with tiny effects that when put together can have an impact,鈥 Gustavson said.

For the study, the researchers used genetic data from nearly 1 million individuals gathered via large datasets like 23 and Me to give each of the adult twins a polygenic score based on their own DNA, for cognitive ability.

Remarkably, the twins鈥 scores closely matched what would be expected based on their tests when they were babies.

鈥淪tudies like ours show us that both family-based and genomic-based datasets are valuable in answering questions about how genetic and environmental influences change across the lifespan,鈥 said Gustavson.